Eastern Fox Squirrels and Humans have the PNMT gene but React Differently to Potential Predator Stimuli
By: Jack Gorno, Nicholas Yeager , Ashley Kimmel, Lindsie Egedy
LB 144 Cell and Molecular Biology
Thursday 11:30 A.M.
Kaleb Howard and Samantha Thacker
11/23/16
https://msu.edu/~kimmelas/
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=KeyOKpHafcY&feature=youtu.be
Finalized by: Jack Gorno
Finalized by: Jack Gorno
Methods
Finalized by: Nicholas Yeager
Finalized by: Ashley Kimmel
References
Finalized by: Jack Gorno
Anonymous. 2016. HUGO Gene Nomenclature Committee Web Site. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/gene/5409, last accessed 9/21/16.
Baron, R.A. and D.R. Richardson. 2004. Aggression: Definition and Perspectives. Pp. 1-11 in Human Aggression. Springer Science & Business Media.
Batter, D.K., S.R. D'Mello, L.M. Turzai, H.B. 3rd Hughes, A.E. Gioio, and B.B. Kaplan. 1988. The complete nucleotide sequence and structure of the gene encoding bovine phenylethanolamine N-methyltransferase. J Neurosci Res 19: 367-376.
Cavallini, P. and J. A. J. Nel. 1990. Ranging Behaviour of the Cape grey mongoose Galerella pulverulenta in a coastal area. Journal of Zoology 222: 353-362.
Davey, G. C. L. 1994. The "Disgusting" Spider: The Role of Disease and Illness in the Perpetuation of Fear of Spiders. Society & Animals. 2: 17-25.
Hoehe, M. R., R. Plaetke, B. Otterud, D. Stauffer, J. Holik, W. F. Byerley, E. E. Baetge, E. S. Gershon, J. M. Lalouel, and M. Leppert. 1992. Genetic linkage of the human gene for phenylethanolamine N-methyltransferase (PNMT), the adrenaline-synthesizing enzyme, to DNA markers on chromosome 17q21-q22. Human Molecular Genetics 1: 175-178.
Keller, M. R. 2014. Fox Squirrel Response to Bark Calls. Ph.D. thesis, Butler University.
Lang, P. J. , M. Davis, and A. Ohman. 2000. Fear and anxiety: animal models and human cognitive psychophysiology. Journal of Affective Disorders 61: 137-159.
Lashley, K. S. 1938. Experimental analysis of instinctive behavior. Psychological Review, Vol 45: 445-471.
McCleery, R.A. 2009. Changes in fox squirrel anti-predator behaviors across the urban-rural gradient. Landscape Ecology 24: 483
McCleery, R. A., R. R. Lopez, N. J. Silvy, and S. N. Kahlick. 2007. Habitat Use of Fox Squirrels in an Urban Environment. The Journal of Wildlife Management: 1149-1157.
McCleery, R. A., R. R. Lopez, N. J. Silvy, and D. L. Gallant. 2008. Fox Squirrel Survival in Urban and Rural Environments. Journal of Wildlife Management 72: 133-137.
Misslin, R. 2003. The defense system of fear: behavior and neurocircuitry. Neurophysiologie Clinique/Clinical Neurophysiology 46: 157-236.
Nunes, S. 2014. Maternal experience and territorial beheavior in ground squirrels. Journal of Mammalogy 95: 491-502.
Panksepp, J. 2000. Affective Consciousness and the Instinctual Motor System. Pp. 27-54 in The Caldron of Consciousness: Motivation, Affect, and Self-organization: an Anthology The Caldron of Consciousness: Motivation, Affect, and Self-organization: an Anthology. John Benjamins Publishing Company.
Phelps, E. A. 2005. Emotion and Cognition: Insights from Studies of the Human Amygdala. Annual Review of Psychology 57: 27-53.
Reilly, T. J. 2007. Squirrelly Behavior. Pest Control 66: 21.
Romero, D. G., M. Y. Zhou, L. L. Yanes, M. W. Plonczynski, T. R. Washington, C. E. Gomez- Sanchez, and E. P. Gomez-Sanchez. 2007. Regulators of G-protein signaling 4 in adrenal gland: localization, regulation, and role in aldosterone secretion. Journal of Endocrinology 194: 429-40.
Roscoe, J.T. and J.A. Bryars. 1971. An Investigation of the Restraints with Respect to Sample Size Commonly Imposed on the Use of the Chi-square Statistic. Journal of American Statistical Association 66: 755-759.
Salsbury, C. M. 2008. Distribution Patterns of Sciurus niger (Eastern Fox Squirrel) Leaf Nests Within Woodlots Across a Suburban/Urban Landscape. Northeastern Naturalist 15: 485-496.
Schwagmeyer, P. and C.H. Brow. 1981. Conspecific Reaction to Playback of Thirteen-lined Ground Squirrel Vocalization. Z. Tierpsychol 56: 25- 32.
Slos S. and R. Stoks. 2008. Predation risk induces stress proteins and reduces antioxidant defense. Functional Ecology 22: 637-642.
Soeter, M. and M. Kindt 2010. Dissociating response systems: Erasing fear from memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 94: 30-41.
Steele, M. A., S. Manierre, T. Genna, T. A. Contreras, P. D. Smallwood, and M. E. Pereira. 2006. The innate basis of food-hoarding decisions in grey squirrels: evidence for behavioural adaptations to the oaks. Animal Behaviour 71: 155-160.
Stein, R. C. 1961. Red Tail Hawk, Macaulay Library 4177.
Taylor, S. E., L. C. Klein, B. P. Lewis,T. L. Gruenewald, R. A. Gurung, J. A. Updegraff. 2000. Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: Tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychological Review 107: 411-429.
Thorson J.M., R. A. Morgan, J. S. Brown, and J. E. Norman. 1998. Direct and indirect cues of predatory risk and patch use by fox squirrels and thirteen-lined ground squirrels. Behavioral Ecology 9: 151-157.
Ulagaraj, S. M., and T. J. Walker. 1973. Phonotaxis of crickets in Flight: Attraction of Male and Female Crickets to Male Calling Songs. Science Vol 182: 1278-1279.
Figures
Finalized by: Lindsie Egedy
Figure 1- (A) Test sites for squirrel and human trials. These locations were selected because of similar human traffic flow and tree density in all areas. Sites L1, L2, and L3 were used for fox squirrel trials. L1 was the Brody complex, L2 was the field between Jenison Field House and the Drayton McLane Baseball Stadium, and L3 is the spillway behind the library. L4 was used for the human experiment and it was the back entrance to MSU's main library. (B) Comparison of fox squirrel reactions to bobcat playback at the three test sites. A stereo played a bobcat growl to simulate an off-campus predator. This was repeated at three locations with 5 trials at each site. Observations were taken of individual squirrel's behavior when the bobcat sound was played. Observations were categorized as response and no response. The test sites proved to be statistically the same with a X^2 value of 2.143 and a p-value of 0.3425 (p>0.05). (C) Comparison of fox squirrel reactions to red-tailed hawk playback at the three test sites. A stereo played a red-tailed hawk cry to simulate an on-campus predator. This was repeated at three locations with 5 trials at each site. Observations were taken of individual squirrel's behavior when the red-tailed hawk sound was played. Observations were categorized as response and no response. The test sites proved to be statistically the same with a X^2 value of 2.143 and a p-value of 0.3425 (p>0.05). (D) Comparison of fox squirrel reactions to the control. A stereo was placed in the site locations with no sound. This was repeated at three locations with 5 trials at each site. Observations were noted for individual squirrel's behavior in the presence of the stereo. Observations were categorized as response and no response. The test sites proved to be statistically the same with a X^2 value of 0 and a p-value of 1 (p>0.05).
Figure 2- Comparison of fox squirrel reactions to hawk and bobcat playback calls. This is a cumulation of the three test sites (L1, L2, and L3) because
they are statistically the same. We used a stereo to play an off-campus bobcat growl and on-campus red-tailed hawk call. We did 5 trials at each location,
which adds up to 15 trials for each predator call. Observations of individual squirrel behaviors were taken when a call was played for 30 seconds. Observations
were categorized as response and no response. The response to both the off-campus and on-campus predators were statistically the same with a X^2 value of 0 and
a p-value of 1 (p>0.05).
Figure 3- Percent of fox squirrel reactions to predator playback compared to the control. This is a cumulation of the predator playback at three test sites because they are statistically the same. The stereo was used to play the bobcat growl and red-tailed hawk cry. In the control, the speaker with no sound was used to ensure the squirrels weren't reacting to the speaker. Observations of response or no response were taken of the squirrel's behavior during playback or no sound. Using a chi-square test, we prove the predator call was statistically different from our control with no sound. The X^2 value was 26.25 and the p-value was 3E-7 (p<0.05). Predator playback and our control were statistically different.
Figure 4- Comparisons of human reactions to a potential threat of a fake spider to no spider. A fake spider was placed on the sidewalk leading into the back entrance of MSU's main library (L4). Observations of response and no response were taken of people who noticed the spider. In the control, with no spider present, observations of people walking in and out of the library were taken. The X^2 value was 10.588 and a p-value of 0.0011 (p<0.05) meaning the potential threat and control were statistically different.
Figure 5- A documentary of learned vs. instinctual anti-predator behaviors for squirrels and humans.
Appendix