Urbanization is the process in which a city acquires more residents, thus increasing the population density (McCleery, 2009). However, when a city becomes urban, humans are not the only organisms that adapt to city life, animals must adapt as well. Amongst the prevalent animals in the city, a prevalent species to Michigan State's campus in East Lansing, Michigan is the Eastern Fox Squirrel. A behavioral difference of urban squirrels compared to rural squirrels have been frequently observed, this behavior is correlated to the accustomization of living in such close proximity to humans and adapting to humans as a stimuli (McCleery, 2009). A predominant reason for having their natural anti-predator behavior reduced is natural selection. The benefits outweigh the costs of having anti-predator behaviors (Blumstein, 2002). With such behaviors diminished the Fox squirrels will have more time to do activities that are important to survival, such as foraging for food (Blumstein and Daniel, 2005).
The Eastern Fox Squirrel is an exceptionally common mammal in Michigan. This species of squirrel is diurnal and reside in areas ranging from rural to urban domains (McCleery, 2009). Their food resources include a variety of hard shell nuts, which depend on abundance and digestibility; walnuts, acorns, and hickory nuts are their main consumption (Smith and Follmer, 1972).They also have a defense mechanism that can help protect them against predators. Defense mechanisms can entail alarm calls, and motor behaviors such as running or freezing. This innate behavior is called, anti-predator behavior. When defense mechanisms are being used, the squirrel stops its activity at hand and becomes alert by freezing, listening, and scanning the area (McCleery, 2009). It is similar to a human's fight-or-flight instinct. However, as previously stated, squirrels who live in an urban environment have scant anti-predator behavior, it is believed that this is a result of the habituation of the species (Parker et al., 2008). The behavior we are specifically looking at is the difference in reactions to threats based on the environment. We believe that humans and squirrels will have similar behavior patterns based on similar genome structure, twelve common chromosomes (Stanyon et. al, 2002). For example, we believe that humans living in a rural setting may be more on edge, attentive, and responsive to threatening stimuli. This may be due to the low amount of stimuli in rural environments compared to an urban environment. Interestingly enough, even seemingly little interference can completely change the responses of individuals to stimuli (McDonnell and Pickett, 1990). In connection to this, humans and squirrels both have a fight or flight response which describes the way in which an individual response to stressful situations. It's believed that vigilance directly corresponds with compromise between risk and energy (Frid and Dill, 2002). Studies have shown that environments have an influence on the modification of mammal's DNA (Provencal, 2011). For example, humans in rural area are not as exposed to a constant source of stressful stimuli, this results in, humans living in a rural environment appearing to demonstrate a greater fear response to stressful stimuli than those humans living in an urban setting that are consistently exposed. This phenomenon could be explained by a concept known as epigenetics (Provencal, 2011). Epigenetics, the study of a change in phenotype without altering the genotype, has shown that although genes will not be altered they can be modified from environmental influences. Stressful or high stimuli environments have shown a modification in the epigenome in which a mark has been left on a specific gene. Researchers have found that this mark influences proteins that produce hormones. The hormones released are responsible for specific stress responses and social behavior (Provencal, 2011).
Vulnerability of squirrels can be concerning as this diminishes anti-predator instinct, this could affect the environment around us and allow the evolution of the commonly known Eastern Fox squirrels (Boonstra et al., 2007. Blumstein and Daniel, 2005. McCleery, 2009) . Evolution through natural selection shows the adaptation of a species to increase survival rates (Boonstra et al., 2007). When food resources become scarce, natural selection will take over to prioritize a species foraging for survival over predatory instincts. Without the consumption of food a species cannot thrive and therefore will begin to die off due to starvation and malnutrition, to avoid this they will diminish their vigilance instinct since predators only provoke a risk of fatality (Walters and Jaunes, 1993).When threatened by a predator the natural instinct of an animal would be to "fight or flight" (McCleery, 2009). By observing their surroundings squirrels can judge the extent of a potential threat. This judgment will allow them to choose the appropriate response which may include, running, or remaining in a motionless state (McCleery, 2009). This behavior is encoded within any animal's natural response system. Is there a way this instinct could be reduced? In a squirrel's natural environment, predators such as humans are scarce and food will be of abundance, yet when faced with limited resources and constant human population, urban environments provide a challenge for foraging squirrels. This challenge has the possibility of imposing a change to the predator instincts of squirrels in order to survive. Previous studies of urban animals and rural animals have shown a distinct contrast in the natural predator instinct. Urbanized animals, such as the Eastern Fox squirrel, showed a decreased response toward traditional predators (Ward and Low, 1997. And McCleery, 2009). The constant exposure to humans in an urbanized environment have decreased the "flight" response of squirrels (Ward and Low, 1997). When the "flight" instinct is diminished this allows for squirrels to continue foraging, without the risk of losing their limited resource. Studies have found that constant exposure to stressful stimuli can physically modify DNA (Provencal, 2011). When exposed to stressful stimuli hormones are released which allow the body to appropriately react. These hormones are regulated by peptides and are found in the pituitary gland. One hormone that is released during a stress response is Adrenocorticotropic Hormone (ACTH) this release then begins the release of glucocorticoids (Boonstra et al., 2007). The gene that controls said hormone releases the Pro-opiomelanocortin (POMC), which is located on chromosome 2, or more specifically 2p23.3 (NIH, 2016). The POMC gene has been identified in both squirrels and humans.
In order to observe how habituation of Eastern Fox squirrels behavior, we plan to use playback sounds to mimic possible threats of danger, such as a car horn or a hawk call, to invoke a physical response from a sample of both urbanized and rural squirrel populations. In addition, once the playback is sounded we will time how long until a response is shown. As in a previous study conducted by researcher Kimberly Jayne, focusing on grey squirrel's response to danger signals while foraging, we want to directly compare how long it takes for a squirrel to respond to direct cues of danger and then relate that to the environment they live in (Jayne et al, 2015). We plan to apply similar methods from this previous study conducted by Jayne, such as the idea of simulating social risk by using acoustic playbacks of stimuli that the squirrels may be exposed to at a foraging patch (Jayne et al, 2015). These methods were chosen because they allow for an observational response without altering any part of the squirrel's environment. While likewise, answering our overall question by providing numerical data that may, later be analyzed to determine if there is in fact a correlation between foraging environment and a reduced response to predator cues. In expansion,the purpose of the purely observational study conducted on humans in order to determine if there is a possible link between vigilance behaviors found in both Eastern Fox squirrels and people, due to a genetic component (Stanyon et al, 2002) . The purpose of the conducted observations is to determine if the environment of a species diminishes their anti-predator instincts. We hypothesize that the urbanization of an environment will diminish anti-predator vigilance instincts to allocate more time for foraging.
Sampling Sites
Written by: Jordan Fournier
Revised by: Jacob Trowbridge
Finalized by: Jacob Trowbridge
Eastern Fox Squirrel Playback
Written by: Kaitlyn Thrush
Revised by: Jacob Trowbridge
Finalized by: Jacob Trowbridge
Written by: Nicole Stewart
Revised by: Jacob Trowbridge
Finalized by:Jacob Trowbridge
Human Observation
Written by: Nicole Stewart
Revised by: Nicole Stewart
Finalized by:Jacob Trowbridge
Statistical Analysis
Written by: Jacob Trowbridge
Revised by: Nicole Stewart
Finalized by: Kaitlyn Thrush
Squirrel experiment
Written by: Nicole Stewart
Finalized by: Kaitlyn Thrush
Written by: Jordan Fournier
Finalized by: Kaitlyn Thrush
Human experiment
Written by: Jacob Trowbridge
Finalized by: Kaitlyn Thrush
Human results
Written by: Kaitlyn Thrush
Finalized by: Kaitlyn Thrush
Finalized by: Nicole Stewart
Boonstra, Rudy., J.M. Baker, J Castillo, and Q.E. Fletcher. 2007. The Role of Stress Axis in Life-History Adaptations. Pp140-141 in Jerry O. Wolff and PaulW.Sherman, eds., Rodent Societies An Ecological & Evolutionary Perspective. The University of Chicago Press Chicago and London.
Daniel T. Blumstein, Jodie G. Ardron, Christopher S. Evans. 2002. Kin discrimination in a macropod marsupial. Ethology. Issue 9. 108: 815-823.
Daniel T Blumstein and Janice C Daniel. 2005. The loss of anti-predator behaviour following isolation on islands.Proc. R. Soc. B. 272: 1663-1668.
Evans, Gary W.1982. General Introduction. Pp. 3-4 in Gary W Evans, eds, Environmental Stress. Cambridge University Press, New York.
Frid, A. and L. M. Dill. 2002. Human-caused disturbance stimuli as a form of predation risk. Conservation Ecology 6(1): 11.
Jerison, Harry J. 1965. Human and Animal Vigilance. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 21: 580-582.
Kimberley Jayne, Stephen E.G. Lea, Lisa A. Leaver. 2015. Behavioural responses of
Eastern grey squirrels, Sciurus carolinensis, to cues of risk while foraging. Centre for Research in Animal Behaviour 116: 53-56. McCloskey R.J. 1975. Description and analysis of the behavior of the fox squirrel in Iowa. thesis, Iowa State University, 33-38.
McDonnell, M.1.,and S. T.A.Pickett.1990.Ecosystem structure and function along urban-rural gradients:an un-exploited opportunity for ecology. Ecology 71: 1232-1237. Milgram, S. 1970. The experience of living in the cities. Science, 167, 1461-1468.
Parker, T.S. & Nilon, C.H. Urban Ecosystems. 2008. Gray squirrel density, habitat suitability, and behavior in urban parks. 11: 243.
Provencal, Nadine., 2011. Beyond Genes: Epigenetics Can Stress Modify our Genes?. Mammoth Magazine Issue 9
http://www.humanstress.ca/documents/pdf/Mammouth%20Magazine/Mammoth_vol9_EN.pdf Robert A. Mccleery. 2009. Changes in fox squirrel antipredator behaviors across the urban-rural gradient. Landscape Ecology. 24:483.
Selye, Hans. 1950. Stress And The General Adaptation Syndrome. British Medical Journal.
Smith, Christopher C. and David Follmer. 1972. Food Preferences of Squirrels. Ecology (Durham). Issue 1. 53: 82-91.
Stanyon, Roscoe., Gary Stone., Monserrat Garcia., Lutz Froenicke. 2002. Reciprocal Chromosome Painting Shows That Squirrels, Unlike Murid Rodents, Have A Highly Conserved Genome Organization. Genomics. Issue 2. 82: 245-249. Walters, C.J., and F. juanes. 1993. Recruitment limitation as a consequence of natural selection for use of restricted feeding habitats and predation risk taking by juvenile fishes. Can. 1. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 58: 2058-2070. Ward C, Low BS (1997) Predictors of vigilance for American crows foraging in the urban environment. WilsonBull 109:481,489.
Wohlwill, J.F. 1970. The concept of sensory overload. Division of Man-Environment relations, The Pennsylvania State University. 340-345
Figure 1. Percent of Time Eastern Fox Squirrels in Both Rural and Urban Environments Spent Foraging and Exhibiting an Alert Behavior. Time percentages of foraging in the observation radius and exhibiting any of the five observed alert behaviors. The total amount of observed time was split into two categories, time spent foraging in the observation radius and time spent exhibiting an alert behavior. The urban environment comprises both urban observations, North Neighborhood and the courtyard by the Union on Michigan State University's campus in East Lansing, MI. The rural environment is compromised by the Sanford Natural Area and the Baker Woodlot on Michigan State University's campus in East Lansing, MI. An inverse correlation between the percent of time spent foraging and the percent of time spent exhibiting an alert behavior is shown between the two environments. Error bars represent one standard error. Written by: Kaitlyn Thrush Finalized by: Jordan Fournier
Figure 2. Alert Behaviors Exhibited by Eastern Fox Squirrels Based on Calls.Scatter plot for the percent of squirrels in each environment, urban and rural, which exhibited an alert behavior in response to the Red-Tail hawk and car horn predatory calls. The rural squirrels exhibited a greater response than the urban squirrels. In the observed rural squirrels a greater response was shown for their natural predator, the Red-Tail hawk. The observed urban squirrels showed a greater response in their unnatural predator, the car horn. The error bars represent one standard error. The data for people interaction was not collect and therefor not applicable. The squirrels were observed on the campus of Michigan State University in East Lansing, MI. Written by: Jordan Fournier Finalized by: Jordan Fournier
Figure 3. Percentages of the most commonly observed squirrel alert reactions. The postures exhibited by the quantified data of all predatory calls performed, Red-Tail hawk and car horn, in both environments, urban and rural. The results presented five different postures exhibited by the observed squirrels. Posture one is indicated by the squirrel on its hind legs, hunched over. Posture two is presented when the squirrel is straight up on its hind legs. A squirrel demonstrates posture three by placing its feet on the base of the tree with its paws pointed upward on the trunk of the tree, the squirrel is looking up in this posture. Posture four is indicated by the squirrel facing down the trunk of the tree with its paws toward the ground and its stomach on the trunk of the tree. The final posture is posture five which is demonstrated by the squirrel laying on a tree branch, with its paws gripped to the underside of the tree and it’s hind legs at the point where the trunk and branch join together. The response of no alert posture exhibited when a predatory call was played was recorded as well. Written by: Jacob Trowbridge Finalized by: Jordan Fournier
Figure 4. The Average Number of Times a Person Looks at a Crosswalk Before Crossing in Rural and Urban Environments. Data has been represented through averages of each environments, rural and urban. The urban location include North Neighborhood and the courtyard by the Union on Michigan State University's campus in East Lansing, MI. The rural location includes the Sanford Natural Area and Baker Woodlot on Michigan State University's campus in East Lansing, MI. The total number of looks was divided by the total number of individuals observed. Urban individuals looked on average 1.83 times before crossing the road where there was no signal, while rural individuals looked on average 1.67 times before crossing. The error bars indicated on the graph show that the urban and rural areas are not significantly different. The vigilance instinct towards vehicles in humans doesn't differ between the environments. Error bars represent one standard error.
Written by: Nicole Stewart Finalized by: Jordan Fournier
Figure 5. Squirrel Habituation Documentary Film.